A1 Vertaisarvioitu alkuperäisartikkeli tieteellisessä lehdessä

The relationship between temperament, polygenic score for intelligence and cognition: A population-based study of middle-aged adults




TekijätTölli Pekka, Keltikangas-Järvinen Liisa, Lehtimäki Terho, Ravaja Niklas, Hintsanen Mirka, Ahola-Olli Ari, Pahkala Katja, Kähonen Mika, Hutri-Kähonen Nina, Laitinen Tomi T, Tossavainen Päivi, Taittonen Lena, Dobewall Henrik, Jokinen Eero, Raitakari Olli, Cloninger C Robert, Rovio Suvi, Saarinen Aino

KustantajaWILEY

Julkaisuvuosi2022

JournalGenes, Brain and Behavior

Tietokannassa oleva lehden nimiGENES BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Lehden akronyymiGENES BRAIN BEHAV

Artikkelin numero e12798

Vuosikerta21

Numero4

Sivujen määrä12

ISSN1601-1848

DOIhttps://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12798

Verkko-osoitehttps://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12798

Rinnakkaistallenteen osoitehttps://research.utu.fi/converis/portal/detail/Publication/174885218


Tiivistelmä
We investigated whether temperament modifies an association between polygenic intelligence potential and cognitive test performance in midlife. The participants (n = 1647, born between 1962 and 1977) were derived from the Young Finns Study. Temperament was assessed with Temperament and Character Inventory over a 15-year follow-up (1997, 2001, 2007, 2012). Polygenic intelligence potential was assessed with a polygenic score for intelligence. Cognitive performance (visual memory, reaction time, sustained attention, spatial working memory) was assessed with CANTAB in midlife. The PGSI was significantly associated with the overall cognitive performance and performance in visual memory, sustained attention and working memory tests but not reaction time test. Temperament did not correlate with polygenic score for intelligence and did not modify an association between the polygenic score and cognitive performance, either. High persistence was associated with higher visual memory (B = 0.092; FDR-adj. p = 0.007) and low harm avoidance with higher overall cognitive performance, specifically better reaction time (B = -0.102; FDR-adj; p = 0.007). The subscales of harm avoidance had different associations with cognitive performance: higher "anticipatory worry," higher "fatigability," and lower "shyness with strangers" were associated with lower cognitive performance, while the role of "fear of uncertainty" was subtest-related. In conclusion, temperament does not help or hinder one from realizing their genetic potential for intelligence. The overall modest relationships between temperament and cognitive performance advise caution if utilizing temperament-related information e.g. in working-life recruitments. Cognitive abilities may be influenced by temperament variables, such as the drive for achievement and anxiety about test performance, but they involve distinct systems of learning and memory.

Ladattava julkaisu

This is an electronic reprint of the original article.
This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Please cite the original version.





Last updated on 2024-26-11 at 20:52